Skip to main content

Full text of "DTIC ADA423616: Anxiety Sensitivity, Body Vigilance, Interoceptive Acuity, and Cardiovascular Reactivity in the Genesis of Panic"

See other formats


UNIFORMED SERVICES UNIVERSITY OF THE HEALTH SCIENCES 

4301 JONES BRIDGE ROAD 
BETHESOA. MARYLAND 20814^799 



APPROVAL SHEET 

Title of Thesis: “Anxiety Sensitivity, Body Vigilance, Interoceptive Acuity, and 

Cardiovascular Reactivity in the Genesis of Panic” 

Name of Candidate: Julie M. Storey, 

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology 
Master of Science 
2000 


Thesis and Abstract Approved: 

Michael Feuerstein, Ph.D. 

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology 
Thesis Advisor 



David Krantz, Ph.D. 

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology 
Committee Member 


Date 


Wendy Law, Eb.D^ ^ 

Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology 
Committee Member 



Printed on 


Recycled Piper 


Report Documentation Page 

Form Approved 

0MB No. 0704-0188 

Public reporting burden for the collection of information is estimated to average 1 hour per response, including the time for reviewing instructions, searching existing data sources, gathering and 
maintaining the data needed, and completing and reviewing the collection of information. Send comments regarding this burden estimate or any other aspect of this collection of information, 
including suggestions for reducing this burden, to Washington Headquarters Services, Directorate for Information Operations and Reports, 1215 Jefferson Davis Highway, Suite 1204, Arlington 

VA 22202-4302. Respondents should be aware that notwithstanding any other provision of law, no person shall be subject to a penalty for failing to comply with a collection of information if it 
does not display a currently valid 0MB control number. 

1. REPORT DATE 

2000 

2. REPORT TYPE 

N/A 

3. DATES COVERED 

4. TITLE AND SUBTITLE 




5a. CONTRACT NUMBER 

"Anxiety Sensitivity, Body Vigilence, Interoceptive Acuity, and 

5b. GRANT NUMBER 

rvciicuvii^ iii uic ui rmiic 


5c. PROGRAM ELEMENT NUMBER 

6. AUTHOR(S) 




5d. PROJECT NUMBER 





5e. TASK NUMBER 





5f. WORK UNIT NUMBER 

7. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 

Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences 4301 Jones Bridge 
Road Bethesda, MD 20814-4799 

8. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION 

REPORT NUMBER 

9. SPONSORING/MONITORING AGENCY NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 


10. SPONSOR/MONITOR’S ACRONYM(S) 





11. SPONSOR/MONITOR’S REPORT 
NUMBER(S) 

12. DISTRIBUTION/AVAILABILITY STATEMENT 

Approved for public release, distribution unlimited 

13. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTES 

14. ABSTRACT 

15. SUBJECT TERMS 

16. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF: 

17. LIMITATION OF 
ABSTRACT 

uu 

18. NUMBER 

OF PAGES 

45 

19a. NAME OF 
RESPONSIBLE PERSON 

a. REPORT 

unclassified 

b. ABSTRACT 

unclassified 

c. THIS PAGE 

unclassified 


Standard Form 298 (Rev. 8-98} 

Prescribed by ANSI Std Z39-18 





The author hereby certifies that the use of any copyrighted materials in the thesis 
manuscript entitled: 

^‘Anxiety Sensitivity, Body Vigilance, Interoceptive Acuity, and Cardiovascular 
Reactivity in the Genesis of Panic” 

beyond brief exerpts is with the permission of the copyright owner and will save and hold 
harmless the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences from any damage 
which may arise from such copyright violations. 


Julie-J^. Storey, CaptHlSA^C^^^ 

Department of Medical and CliniCaHP^ychology 
Uniformed Services University of the Health 


Sciences 


11 



ABSTRACT 


Title of Thesis: Anxiety Sensitivity, Body Vigilance, Interoceptive Acuity, and 
Cardiovascular Reactivity in the Genesis of Panic 
Julie M. Storey, Master of Science, 2000 

Thesis directed by: Michael Feuerstein, Ph.D., Professor, Department of Medical and 
Clinical Psychology 

Cognitive conceptualizations of panic require both the experience of arousal symptoms 
and their catastrophic interpretation. The tendency to interpret arousal symptoms as 
threatening is known as anxiety sensitivity (AS), but it is unclear if increased vigilance, 
greater physiological reactivity, or enhanced perception are responsible for reported 
physiological symptoms. Each of these mechanisms has been empirically supported in 
clinical, but not nonclinical, populations. The current investigation examined the ability 
of AS, body vigilance, cardiovascular reactivity, and interoceptive acuity to predict 
fearful responding to a 35% CO 2 inhalation in a nonclinical population. A main effect 
was found for AS (R^ = .13; p < .01). Two interaction effects were found (AS x Heart 
Rate, AR^ = .05, p < .05; AS x Diastolic Blood Pressure, AR^ = .05, p < .05). Results 
support cognitive theories of panic and suggest physiological reactivity combined with 
AS elicit more fearful responses than either alone. 



ANXIETY SENSITIVITY, BODY VIGILANCE, 
INTEROCEPTIVE ACUITY, AND CARDIOVASCULAR 
REACTIVITY IN THE GENESIS OF PANIC 


by 

Julie M. Storey, Capt, USAF 


Thesis submitted to the Faculty of the 
Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology Graduate Program of the 
Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences 
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of 
Master of Science 2000 


IV 



TABLE OF CONTENTS 

Approval Sheet. i 

Copyright Statement. ii 

Abstract. Hi 

Title. iv 

Table of Contents. v 

List of Tables. vi 

List of Figures. vii 

Introduction. 1 

Physical Symptoms in Panic. 1 

Body Vigilance . 2 

Interoceptive Acuity . 4 

Cardiovascular Reactivity . 6 

Catastrophic Interpretation of Symptoms in Panic. 9 

Study Hypotheses. 11 

Methods. 12 

Sample and Procedures. 12 

Physiological Measures. 14 

Psychological Measures. 15 

Biological Challenge Procedure. 16 

Protocol Overview. 17 

Data Analytic Strategy. 18 

Results. 19 


V 

























Descriptive Statistics. 19 

Missing Data. 20 

Main Effects. 21 

Interaction Effects. 22 

Discussion. 23 

Limitations of the Study. 26 

Conclusions.27 

References. 29 


vi 











LIST OF TABLES 


Table 1: Contact Results for 1516 Phone Numbers 
Table 2; Descriptive Statistics for the Sample 
Table 3: Descriptive Statistics for the Predictor Measures 
Table 4; Correlations Among Predictor Variables 

Table 5: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Post 
CO 2 Fear 

Table 6: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Cross Products Predicting 
Post CO 2 Fear 


vu 



LIST OF FIGURES 


Figure 1: Interaction Between Anxiety Sensitivity and Heart Rate Reactivity for 
Predicting Post CO 2 Fear 

Figure 2: Interaction Between Anxiety Sensitivity and Diastolic Blood Pressure 
Reactivity for Predicting Post CO 2 Fear 



Introduction 


The central features of a panic attack, as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical 
Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-FV; American Psychiatric 
Association, 1994), are intense fear accompanied by somatic and/or cognitive symptoms. 
This theoretically singular entity is estimated to affect 28-34% of individuals across their 
lifetimes (Norton, Harrison, Hauch, and Rhodes, 1985) and is linked to a broad range of 
psychological and physical outcomes. While some individuals report an isolated, 
uncomfortable event, others report a debilitating disorder (e.g., panic disorder). 

Since the inclusion of panic attacks and panic disorder in the DSM-III (Diagnostic 
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition; Ajnerican Psychiatric 
Association, 1980), this amalgam of physical and emotional complaints has been a topic 
of much research and debate. Significant advances have been made in our understanding 
of the maintenance and clinical course of these entities (McNally, 1994). Cognitive 
theory, applied to panic attacks and panic disorder in the mid-1980’s, proved to be an 
important impetus for panic research. Cognitive conceptualizations of panic suggest that 
a panic attack results when benign physical symptoms are catastrophically interpreted 
(Clark, 1986; Reiss and McNally, 1985). Two key components identified by this 
conceptualization are: the experience of physical symptoms, and the interpretation of 
them in a catastrophic framework. 

Physical Symptoms in Panic 

Much debate abounds regarding the actual and perceived physical symptoms 
“experienced” by individuals plagued by panic attacks. Ehlers (1993) identified three 
hypotheses regarding increased symptom perception in panic disorder. Patients with 


1 



2 


panic disorder may be more apt to perceive autonomic sensations because of: increased 
attention to levels of physiological arousal; enhanced ability to perceive normal 
physiological aberrations; and/or greater physiological reactivity to situations 
experienced as anxiogenic. 

Body vigilance. In cognitive theory, a systematic preference for certain types of 
information is known as an attentional bias (Clark and Fairbum, 1997). An attentional 
bias for somatic changes has been suggested and observed to play a role in the perception 
of physical sensations (Ehlers, 1993). Pennebaker, Gonder-Frederick, Cox, and Hoover 
(1985) observed that increased attentional focus on the body increases the likelihood of 
perceiving potentially threatening interoceptive cues. Although this assertion has 
historically been reserved for patients with hypochondriacal concerns (i.e., concerned 
with the onset or presence of serious physical illness), recent evidence suggests it may be 
true for panic disorder patients as well as individuals with subclinical somatic concerns. 

Investigations into panic disorder have labeled this tendency to selectively attend to 
somatic changes as body vi^lance (Schmidt, Lerew, and Trakowski, 1997). In a multi¬ 
study investigation of the relationship between body vigilance and anxiety pathology, 
Schmidt and colleagues (1997) found body vigilance to be normally distributed in a 
nonclinical sample and to be related to a history of spontaneous panic attacks, anxiety 
sensitivity, and anxiety symptomatology. Additionally, individuals with panic disorder 
reported higher levels of body vigilance relative to social phobics and nonclinical 
controls and showed reductions in body vigilance associated with reported reductions in 
panic symptoms following a cognitive-behavioral intervention. 



3 


It has additionally been suggested that this heightened vigilance exists only for 
somatic disturbances related to autonomic nervous system arousal (e.g., heart palpitations 
and shortness of breath) but not for non-autonomic sensations (e.g., muscle aches or 
stomach pains) in panic disorder patients (Pilkington, Antony, and Swinson, 1998). In a 
study involving anxiety disordered patients and nonclinical volunteers, Pilkington and 
colleagues found vigilance for autonomic sensations to be significantly elevated in panic 
disorder patients relative to nonclinical controls, specific phobic patients, social phobic 
patients, and obsessive-compulsive patients. Panic disorder patients were no more 
vigilant for non-autonomic bodily sensations than nonclincal controls. 

These data suggest an attentional bias for symptoms of autonomic arousal in panic 
disordered patients. Evidence for such a bias, and indirect evidence for the relevance of 
body vigilance in the maintenance of panic disorder, can be gleaned from Horenstein and 
Segui (1997) and Asmundson, Sandler, Wilson, and Walker (1992). Both of these 
studies were reaction time studies using panic disorder patients, clinical controls, and 
nonclinical controls. Two words were presented simultaneously to the subject, one word 
was a neutral word, and the other a threatening word. A probe dot then replaced one of 
the words, and the latency to detection of the probe was measured. Panic disorder 
patients were quicker to detect the probe when it replaced the threatening word rather 
than the neutral word. Asmundson et al. (1992) found a similar pattern, but only for 
physically threatening words (e.g., collapse and death). Socially threatening words (e.g., 
failure and stupid) did not enhance reaction time. 

Although a relatively young construct, these preliminary investigations suggest that 
body vigilance to be a behavioral sequela of catastrophic cognitions and a mechanism by 



4 

which individuals with panic disorder detect changes in their somatic states leading to 
self-perpetuating anxiety responses. 

Interoceptive acuity. Whereas body vigilance involves attention to somatic 
perturbations, interoceptive acuity involves accuracy in detecting such changes. 

Although results have been somewhat mixed, some research suggests that panic disorder 
patients, in addition to heightened vigilance, may also have an enhanced accuracy in the 
perception of internal somatic cues (Ehlers and Breuer, 1992). Although empirical 
support is relatively recent, the notion that certain people are more attuned to changes in 
their soma is not a new one. Tyrer (1976) reports that patients with somatic anxiety are 
better at perceiving bodily states than other subjects, and Shands and Schor (1982) 
described panic disorder patients as “interoceptive experts, being able to describe changes 
in almost every organ system and region of the body” (p. 108). Because cardiovascular 
symptoms are common during panic, evaluation of interoceptive acuity in panic disorder 
patients has typically focused on the accuracy of detecting one’s heart rate (Ehlers and 
Breuer, 1992; McLeod and Hoehn-Saric, 1993). 

Early studies of interoception began as studies of vigilance - focusing more on 
patient’s self-reported awareness and not on their objective accuracy. These early 
investigations suggest that panic disorder patients believe they are more aware of somatic 
(usually cardiovascular) aberrations and act accordingly (fearfully) when they “perceive” 
such changes. For example, subjects with high state or trait anxiety have shown greater 
reported cardiac awareness than non-anxious subjects (Schandry, 1981; Montgomery and 
Jones, 1984), and nonclinical subjects who report good heartbeat perception are 
emotionally more responsive to stressors than subjects who do not (Katkin, 1985; 



5 


Schandry, 1983). Ehlers and Breuer (1992) foimd panic patients, as well as infrequent 
panickers, reported greater baseline cardiac sensitivity than either patients with other 
anxiety disorders or nonclinical controls. Additionally, self-reported cardiac awareness 
has been found to be related to the degree of agoraphobic avoidance behavior among 
frequent and infrequent panickers. Anxiety patients, but not healthy volimteers, report 
increases in anxiety and exhibit increases in physiological arousal when they hear false 
auditory feedback indicating a surge in heart rate (Ehlers, Margraf, Roth, Taylor, and 
Birbaumer, 1988). Although such self-reported awareness indirectly suggests heightened 
accuracy, self-reports of interoceptive sensitivity and objective measures of accuracy tend 
to have low correlations (Shands and Schor, 1982; Tyrer, 1973, 1976; Whitehead, 
Drescher, Heiman, and Blackwell, 1977). 

Some studies investigating objective accuracy suggest panic disorder patients are 
more accurate at perceiving their heart beats than patients with simple phobias, infrequent 
panickers, and nonclinical controls, although panic disorder patients are not typically 
completely accurate (Ehlers and Breuer, 1992). Ehlers (1995), in a prospective 
evaluation of panic disorder patients, infrequent panickers, simple phobics, remitted 
panickers, and controls found maintenance of the disorder as well as relapse to be 
associated with good heartbeat perception, in addition to anxiety sensitivity, and degree 
of avoidance. 

Although evidence suggests panic disorder patients have heightened awareness of 
their cardiac sensations, it is unclear whether increased cardiac awareness is present 
before the onset of panic attacks or whether it is acquired later in the course of the 
disorder (Ehlers and Breuer, 1992). It has been hypothesized that interoceptive acuity 



6 


may evolve from frequent pulse taking or changes in attentional focus that increase the 
probability that panic patients will be aware of normal cardiac changes (e.g., 
tachycardias, arrhythmias) that occur frequently in many people but go unnoticed (Ehlers 
and Breuer, 1992). As interoceptive acuity improves, such changes are more likely to be 
detected by vulnerable subjects (i.e., subjects with high anxiety sensitivity), and a panic 
attack is apt to be triggered. At least one study (Ehlers and Breuer, 1992) found 
infrequent panickers to be no better than nonclinical controls in detecting their heart rate, 
suggesting accuracy of heart rate detection, if it in fact exists in panickers, may be a result 
of long-term experience with panic attacks more than a cause. 

Cardiovascular reactivity. Changes from baseline functioning of the heart or 
vasculature induced by experimental or in vivo demands are known as cardiovascular 
reactivity . Largely due to the fact that symptoms frequently reported by anxiety disorder 
patients suggest an increase in cardiovascular functioning (e.g., palpitations, chest pain, 
chest tighmess), the literature is replete with investigations into the relationship between 
cardiovascular measures and anxiety symptomatology. Although results are not entirely 
consistent, a large portion of the evidence suggests baseline cardiovascular profiles, 
which include heart rate and blood pressure measures, of individuals with anxiety 
pathology do not differ significantly from those of nonclinical controls (Braune, Albus, 
Froehler, and Hoen et al., 1994; Hoehn-Saric and McLeod, 1988; Sandler, Wilson, 
Asmundson, Larsen, et al., 1992). However, it appears as individuals with anxiety 
pathology begin to experience anxiety, they may experience larger changes in their 
cardiovascular functions, including greater elevations in heart rate, systolic blood 



7 


pressure and diastolic blood pressure (Hoehn-Saric and McLeod; 1988; Pauli, Marquardt, 
Hartl, Nutzinger, Holzl, and Strian, 1991). 

Heart rate reactivity. Several laboratory studies have demonstrated the presence of 
elevated heart rate responses to physiological challenges in panic disorder patients when 
compared to nonclinical controls. Patients with panic disorder have been shown to 
demonstrate greater heart rate reactivity to treadmill exercise (Taylor, King, Ehlers et al., 
1987), to an orthostatic challenge (Stein, Papp, Klein, et al., 1992), to a biological 
challenge (vanZijderveld, TenVoorde, Veltman, and van Doomen, 1997), and to certain 
psychomotor tasks (Hoehn-Saric, McLeod, and Zimmerli, 1991) when compared with 
nonclinical controls, social phobics (Veltman, van Zijderveld, Tilder, and van Dyck, 
1996), and blood phobics (Friedman, Thayer, Borkoved, Tyrrell, Johnson, Columbo, 

1993; Friedman and Thayer, 1998). As with many constructs implicated in anxiety 
pathology, however, it remains unclear if this increased reactivity is a risk factor for panic 
or a consequence of cognitive (fear) and behavioral (avoidance of exercise) sequelae of 
panic. 

Blood pressure reactivity. Studies of blood pressure reactivity, especially in relation 
to diastolic blood pressure reactivity, have yielded equivocal results. Although most 
investigations report elevated systolic blood pressure reactivity in panic disorder patients 
during ambulatory monitoring and in response to a variety of stressors (e.g., a mental 
stress task, epinephrine challenge; Bystritsky, Craske, Maidenberg, Vapnik, and Shapiro, 
1995; Hoehn-Saric et al., 1991; Shear et al, 1992; White and Baker, 1987; vanZijderveld, 
TenVoorde, Veltman, and van Doomen, 1997), diastolic reactivity differs depending 
upon the type of stressor and the time of measurement (Bystritsky and Shapiro, 1992; 



8 


Yeregani, Meiri, Pohl, Baton, Desai, and Golec, 1990; vanZijderveld, TenVoorde, 
Veltman, and van Dooraen, 1997). However, Bystritsky and Shapiro (1992) reported 
elevated diastolic blood pressure reactivity in panic disorder patients during a carbon 
dioxide challenge, and three different investigations of ambulatory blood pressure 
indicate significantly elevated diastolic blood pressure reactivity prior to and during panic 
in a naturalistic setting (Bystritsky et al, 1995; Shear et al, 1992; White and Baker, 1987). 

Although evidence to date suggests cardiovascular reactivity’s role in the 
maintenance of anxiety pathology, few studies have investigated its role in development 
of these disorders. Sandler, Wilson, Asmundson, Gordon, Larsen et al. (1992) compared 
infrequent panickers to nonpanicking controls and discovered that infrequent panickers 
did not evidence the heightened autonomic activation that is often found in individuals 
with panic disorder. This study, while not conclusive, suggests that cardiovascular 
reactivity may be a systemic response to repeated, intense autonomic arousal and not a 
causal mechanism in the development of panic. To date, no studies have prospectively 
investigated cardiovascular reactivity, and few have reported its relationship to fear in a 
nonclinical sample. Shostak and Peterson (1990) investigated the relationship between 
anxiety sensitivity and physiological changes in nonclinical subjects following a mental 
arithmetic task and found comparable muscle activity and systolic blood pressure across 
three levels of anxiety sensitivity. However, one might logically question the relevance 
of a mental stress task for individuals high in anxiety sensitivity. Similarly, Asmtmdson, 
Norton, Wilson, and Sandler (1994) failed to find heart-rate reactivity differences 
between high and low anxiety sensitive nonclinical subjects following a hyperventilation 
challenge. In this investigation, Asmundson and colleagues failed to provide evidence of 



9 

arousal in either group, bringing into question the efficacy of the stressor and thus the 
acciu'acy of their conclusions. 

Catastrophic Interpretation of Symptoms in Panic 

The second component of the cognitive conceptualization of panic is the 
misinterpretation of innocuous, internal bodily sensations as threatening (Barlow, 1988, 
Clark, 1986). Identified as threatening, these sensations may trigger anxiety, increased 
arousal, and self-perpetuating fear. According to this model of panic, the mere 
experience of physical arousal is insufficient to elicit panic. The individual must believe 
that the physical arousal may have negative consequences. 

The extent to which an individual believes that autonomic arousal can result in 
catastrophic consequences in known as anxiety sensitivity (Reiss and McNally, 1985). 
Individuals high in anxiety sensitivity may believe that shortness of breath signals 
suffocation or that heart palpitations indicate a heart attack, whereas those low in anxiety 
sensitivity experience these sensations as uncomfortable but innocuous (McNally, 1994). 
Evidence suggests that anxiety sensitivity is a stable, trait-like characteristic that emerges 
from experiences (both personal and observational) that yoke aversive consequences with 
arousal (McNally, 1994). 

Empirical support for anxiety sensitivity’s role in panic is fairly strong. Elevated 
anxiety sensitivity has been shown to be typical of anxiety disorders in general (Taylor, 
Koch, and McNally, 1992), with panic disorder patients scoring two standard deviations 
greater than controls, and significantly higher than patients with generalized anxiety 
disorder (McNally, Amir, Louro, Lukach, Riemann, and Calamari, 1994). Additionally, 
anxiety sensitivity has been shown to predict diagnostic severity of panic disorder (Jones 



10 

and Barlow, 1991, c.f., Telch, Silverman, and Schmidt, 1996) and has been shown to 
diminish with the reduction of anxiety symptomatology (McNally and Lorenz, 1987; 
Telch, Lucas, Schmidt, Hanna, Jaimez and Lucas, 1993). 

Empirical evidence garnered via multiple methods and across multiple populations 
suggests anxiety sensitivity may play a prominent role in the pathogenesis and the 
maintenance of panic disorder (McNally et al., 1998). Most notably, prospective 
investigations have identified anxiety sensitivity as a risk factor for the development of 
panic attacks and panic disorder (Donnell and McNally, 1990; Ehlers, 1995; Mailer and 
Reiss, 1992; Schmidt et al., 1997). For example, Ehlers (1995) found high baseline 
anxiety sensitivity to be associated with the occurrence of a first panic attack during a 
one-year follow-up period. In the laboratory, anxiety sensitivity has been found to 
predict emotional and physical responding to various biological challenges regardless of 
panic history and diagnostic status (Donnell and McNally, 1989; Schmidt and Telch, 
1994; Rapee and Medoro, 1994; Telch and Harrington, 1993; Telch et al., 1996; 
Holloway and McNally, 1987). Telch and Harrington (1992) found subjects with high 
anxiety sensitivity without a history of panic attacks exhibit rates of CO 2 -induced panic 
comparable to panic disorder patients. Eke and McNally (1996) found that psychological 
variables reflecting fears of bodily sensations are better predictors of response to carbon 
dioxide challenge than either behavioral sensitivity to carbon dioxide or general trait 
anxiety. 

While research concerning anxiety sensitivity, body vigilance, interoceptive acuity, 
and cardiovascular reactivity has shed considerable light upon the maintenance of panic, 
the focus has only recently turned to the enlightenment of its pathogenesis (Mailer and 



11 


Reiss, 1992; Schmidt, Lerew, and Jackson, 1997; Ehlers and Breuer, 1992; Schmidt et al., 
1997). Additionally, prior investigations into risk factors for anxiety pathology have to 
date involved the sole consideration of the singular effects of each of the suspected risk 
factors. Very few studies have investigated the relationship between anxiety sensitivity, 
cardiac reactivity, and heartbeat perception in nonclinical subjects. 

In one of the few existing investigations into these constructs in a nonclinical 
sample, Sturges and Goetsch (1996) exposed nonclinically anxious (i.e., high anxiety 
sensitivity) women and nonanxious (i.e., low anxiety sensitivity) women to caffeine- 
induced arousal and compared heart rate and skin conductance reactivity and 
interoceptive accuracy and failed to find an effect. However, methodological difficulties, 
such as the failure to measure blood pressure and the allowance of practice trials on the 
heart beat tracking task, may have masked or ignored important differences between the 
groups. The authors also did not investigate interactive effects between the variables of 
interest, and they failed to include a measure of symptoms and subjective distress in 
relation to the caffeine challenge. 

Thus, the principle aim of the present study was to investigate the hypotheses 
proposed by Ehlers (1993) by evaluating the singular and interactive effects of anxiety 
sensitivity, body vigilance, heartbeat perception, and cardiovascular reactivity in 
predicting fearful responding to a biological challenge in individuals with no history of 
spontaneous panic or any anxiety disorder diagnosis. 

Study Hypotheses 

Ehlers’s (1993) hypothesized that individuals with panic disorder are more likely to 
perceive physical symptoms for one of three reasons: they pay more attention to them; 



12 


are more accurate at perceiving them; or they are more reactivity physiologically to 
anxiogenic physical stimuli. Additionally, recent research into the role of cognitions in 
panic suggests the catastrophic interpretation of physical symptoms is important in the 
genesis of anxiety and fear. Therefore, it was hypothesized that: 

- Catastrophic interpretation of symptoms will predict fearful responding to a carbon 
dioxide challenge in a nonclinical sample. 

- Catastrophic interpretation will mediate the roles of somatic vigilance, interoceptive 
acuity, and cardiovascular reactivity in self-reported fear. That is, neither body 
vigilance, heartbeat perception, nor cardiovascular reactivity (i.e., changes in heart 
rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure) will predict fearful responding unless they 
are catastrophically interpreted. 


Methods 

Sample and Procedures 

72 (27 males and 45 females) non-clinical subjects from Washington, D.C. and 
Montgomery County, Maryland participated in the study. Potential subjects were 
contacted by phone from a list of 1,516 phone numbers selected randomly by city block 
and purchased from a national survey sampling company (See Table 1). Of the 1,516 
phone numbers, 107 eligible residences expressing interest in the study were identified. 
These 107 households contained a total of 135 eligible people. 

Respondents were administered a brief, preliminary telephone interview to 
determine their eligibility. The data presented in this paper were collected as part of a 



13 


Table 1 


Contact Results for 1516 Random Phone Numbers 


Status 

Count 

Business (nonresidential) Numbers 

131 

Couldn’t Contact 

639 

Disconnected Numbers 

288 

Fax Numbers 

87 

Hang-ups 

20 

Ineligible Households 

203 

Age: 156 


Medical: S 


Psych Hx: 5 


Other: 37 


Not Interested 

41 

Interested/Eligible households 

107 

Total 

1516 


larger, prospective study of risk factors for anxiety pathology. Based upon 
epidemiological data that suggest young adults to be at highest risk for the development 
of anxiety pathology (Weissman and Merikangas, 1986; Bums and Thorpe, 1977), 
eligible respondents were between the ages of 18 and 35 and without a history of 
spontaneous panic or a DSM-FV Axis I disorder. Additionally, eligible respondents were 
without significant, current medical illness (e.g., renal, cardiovascular, neurological, or 
pulmonary disease), which could accoimt for differences in physiological indices at 
baseline and in response to the biological challenge task. Due to the format of the current 
protocol, all respondents were able to comprehend spoken and written English. All 
eligible individuals living in the selected household were accepted into the study and 
were asked to come in for one lab session. 


14 


Physiological Measures 

Vital capacity. A Respirodyne II Plus respirometer and disposable flow sensors 
were used to measure each subject’s yital capacity (VC). VC is the maximum yolume of 
air that can be moyed in and out of the lungs and is measured in liters. VC was assessed 
three times and ayeraged to yield a VC index. 

Heart rate and blood pressure. Heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure were 
recorded using a Critikon Dynamap Vital Signs Monitor, Model 1846 SX. The monitor 
was set to read blood pressure and heart rate at one-minute interyals during the 10-minute 
baseline. Measures were suspended during the interoception protocol (see below). 
Following each inhalation, 2 immediate blood pressure and heart rate measures were 
taken. 

Vagal tone. A single channel, three lead signal from an electrocardiographic pre- 
amplifler (VTMII, Scope Services, Bethesda, Maryland), proyided yisual, computerized 
numeric output of heart rate which allowed for precise assessment of the subject’s 
interoceptiye accuracy. 

Interoceptiye accuracy. Interoceptiye accuracy was assessed yia the mental tracking 
paradigm designed by Schandry (1981) and modified by Ehlers and Breuer (1992). 
Subjects were instructed to count their heartbeats silently during interyals of 35 seconds, 
25 seconds, and 45 seconds without taking their pulses or using other perception 
strategies. In order to minimize random guessing, subjects were specifically instructed to 
count only the heartbeats they actually perceiye and not to count just because they know 
their heart should be beating (Zoellner and Craske, 1999). Each interval began with the 
experimenter asking if the subject was ready, and upon a positive response stating, “Go.” 



15 


Simultaneously, another experimenter counted actual R-waves from electrocardiogram 
(ECG) recordings of the VTM-II. Interoceptive accuracy was defined as the absolute 
value of the actual number of heartbeats minus the number of perceived heartbeats 
divided by the actual number of heartbeats multiplied by 100. The resulting quotient is 
identified as heart beat accuracy (HBA). 

Psychological Measures 

Structured clinical interview . In order to assess Axis I psychiatric disorders, the 
Structured Clinical Interview for Axis IDSM-IV Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P- 
Version 2.0; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, and Williams, 1996) was administered to all subjects 
by a graduate student trained in its administration. The SCID-I/P has shown high validity 
and good test-retest reliability in diagnosing Axis I disorders in a variety of populations 
(Williams, Spitzer, and Gibbon, 1992). 

Acute Panic Inventory. The Acute Panic Inventory (API) is a 24-item inventory for 
assessing symptoms of arousal associated with panic attacks (Liebowitz et al., 1984). 
Subjects rate the severity of each symptom from 0 (absent) to 3 (severe). Examples 
include, “Did you feel faint?”, and “Were you afraid of dying?”. The API includes two 
self-report visual analog scales of the subject’s current level of fear and highest level of 
fear (0 - no fear at all, 100 - extreme fear). 

Anxiety Sensitivity Index. The Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI) is a 16-item 
questionnaire that measures fear of somatic symptoms related to arousal (Peterson and 
Reiss, 1992). Each item assesses a concern about the possible negative consequences of 
anxiety symptoms on a 0 to 4 point likert scale. This test is scored by summing each 
response to provide a total score. The ASI has demonstrated adequate internal 



16 


consistency (Telch, Shennis, and Lucas, 1989) and test-retest reliability (Mailer and 
Reiss, 1992). The ASI appears to tap into fear of anxiety symptoms as opposed to state 
or trait anxiety (see McNally, 1994). The mean for nonclinical samples has been 
observed to be 19 (Peterson and Reiss, 1992). 

Body Vigilance Scale. The Body Vigilance Scale (BVS; Schmidt et al., 1997) is a 
4-item self-report inventory that was designed to assess attentional focus to internal body 
sensations. The first three items assess degree of attentional focus, perceived sensitivity 
to changes in bodily sensations, and the average amount of time spent attending to bodily 
sensations. The fourth item involves separate ratings for attention to 15 sensations (e.g., 
heart palpitations) that include all of the physical symptoms described for panic attacks in 
the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Scores on item 3 are divided by 
10. Ratings for the 15 sensations are averaged to yield a single score for item 4. The 
BVS total score is the sum of items 1-4. The BVS has been shown to have adequate test- 
retest reliability and high internal consistency, and the mean score for nonclinical 
subjects has been reported to be 18.0 (Schmidt et. al., 1997). 

Biological Challenge Procedure 

Prior to exposing the subject to the CO 2 challenge, the subject’s vital capacity (VC) 
was measured. The experimenter provided instructions and a demonstration of the VC 
procedure. The subject inhaled as much air as possible, placed his/her mouth around the 
flow sensor making a tight seal, and exhaled through the flow sensor. Following three 
VC measurements, the subjects were given the following instructions: “Next you will 
take two deep breaths. One breath will be of normal room air, and the other will be a 
mixture of oxygen and carbon dioxide. The oxygen and carbon dioxide mixture will 



17 


produce some short-lived sensations. You will not be told which one you will receive 
first in order to ensure your response will not be biased. As I said before, you will take 
one single breath of each gas. I will ask you to hold the inhalation while I count to five, 
and then you can exhale.” Next, the experimenter demonstrated taking a vital capacity 
breath from the venti-comp bag. The order of presentation of the two gases was 
determined by coin flip prior to the initiation of the protocol. The experimenter then 
assisted the subject in taking a vital capacity breath of each of the gases (35% carbon 
dioxide and 65% oxygen, and room air). The mixture was delivered to subjects via 4.8 
liter venti-comp bags filled to capacity. The patient, with nostrils closed, exhaled all of 
the air in his/her lungs and then inhaled from the venti-comp bag via a one-way flow 
valve with the goal of inhaling as much of the mixture as possible. The challenge phase 
consisted of the inhalation period plus 30 seconds following COi inhalation. The CO 2 
intake volume was assessed by measuring the amount of CO 2 remaining in the venti- 
comp bag. The API was obtained after each phase. The patient was then disconnected 
from the apparatus. 

Protocol Overview 

After reviewing the consent forms and administering the SCID-I/P, subjects were 
given a battery of self-report inventories including the ASI, BVS, and the STAI. After 
completing these questionnaires, subjects were then connected to the VTM-II and 
dynamap and asked to sit still and quietly for a 10-minute baseline. At the end of the 
baseline period, subjects completed the Interoceptive Accuracy paradigm and then the 
CO 2 challenge. 



18 


Data Analytic Strategy 

Total scores were calculated for the ASI and the BVS according to their 
standardized scoring algorithms. Measures of reactivity for each of the cardiovascular 
indices (heart rate, diastolic blood pressure, and systolic blood pressure) were calculated 
by partialling out the variance in scores accounted for by baseline measures (i.e., for each 
cardiovascular measure, a linear regression analysis was performed using the measure’s 
baseline level and room air reactivity level as independent variables and the CO 2 
reactivity level as the dependent level. The unstandardized residuals of these regressions 
were used as the reactivity variable for each of cardiovascular indices). 

Two hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed to examine the 
predictors of self-reported fear in response to the CO 2 challenge. The dependent variable 
was the subject’s level of fear reported immediately after the biological challenge on the 
visual analog scale of the API. The six independent variables for this analysis 
represented fear of arousal symptoms (ASI) and Ehlers’s (1993) hypothesized 
mechanisms for perception of physical symptoms (BVS, HBA, AHR, ADBP, ASBP). 

The independent variables were entered stepwise, and the order of entry was based on 
cognitive theory and prior research findings discussed in the Introduction. 

The second regression analysis was designed to identify interaction effects 
between the ASI and each of the other five independent variables on the dependent 
variable, post-C02 self-reported level of fear. The independent variables were entered in 
six steps. The first step contained the main effects for ASI, BVS, HBA, AHR, ADBP, 
ASBP. Steps two through six contained the cross-products of the ASI and each of the 



19 


other five variables. The cross products were entered stepwise, and the order of entry 
was based on cognitive theory and prior research findings discussed in the Introduction. 


Results 

Descriptive Statistics 

Table 2 gives descriptive statistics for the initial sample of 72 subjects. The sample 
primarily included young, healthy Caucasian women with at least a high school diploma. 
The sample means on the anxiety sensitivity index (ASI), the body vigilance scale (BVS), 
and baseline cardiovascular measures were within the normal range but were lower than 
previously reported means for nonclinical subjects (Table 2). 


Table 2 


Sample Descriptive Statistics 



n 

% 

Gender 

Female 

45 

62.5% 

Male 

27 

37.5% 

Ethnicity 

Caucasian 

40 

54.9% 

African American 

20 

28.1% 

Hispanic 

5 

7.2% 

Asian 

3 

4.2% 

Other 

4 

5.6% 


Range 

MfSDl 

Age(yrs) 

18-35 

26.9(4.6) 

Education (yrs) 

11-18 

14.7(2.1) 






20 


Missing Data 

As Table 3 indicates, each predictor variable had missing data points. Most of these 
missing data points were due to time constraints and equipment difficulties. None of the 
missing data points were due to subject refusal to complete the protocol, and no single 
subject was missing more than 2 data points. Pairwise deletion of subjects with missing 
data yielded a reduced sample of 48. As a result, statistical analyses included 
replacement of missing data with the sample mean for that measure. 


Table 3 

Descriptive Statistics for the Predictor Measures 


Missing 

data 


Variable 

Range 

MfSDl 

n 

% 

Anxiety Sensitivity Index 

1-37 

14.58(8.2) 

7 

9.7 

Body Vigilance Scale 

0 - 35.3 

10.81(8.2) 

5 

6.9 

Heartbeat Accuracy (% inaccurate) 

3.4 - 100 

87.12(21.0) 

4 

5.5 

Baseline Systolic Blood Pressure 

84-150 

111.53(13.1) 

7 

9.7 

Post CO 2 Systolic Blood Pressure 

91 - 164 

120.48(16.4) 

8 

11.1 

Baseline Diastolic Blood Pressme 

48-92 

66.91(9.1) 

7 

9.7 

Post CO 2 Diastolic Blood Pressure 

52-99 

73.46(10.0) 

8 

11.1 

Baseline Heart Rate 

41-96 

64.76(11.0) 

7 

9.7 

Post CO 2 Heart Rate 

45-104 

67.77(11.4) 

6 

8.3 


Correlation Matrix 


Table 4 contains the correlations among the six independent variables. Significant 
correlations (p<.05) existed between the ASI and SBP reactivity, and SBP reactivity and 
HR reactivity. 






21 


Table 4 

Correlation Matrix for Predictor Variables 


Variable 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

1. ASI 

“ 

.101 

.077 

-.099 

-.270 

.327* 

2. BVS 


“ 

.230 

.035 

-.035 

o 

1 

3. HBA 



-- 

-.163 

-.172 

.082 

4. AHR 




— 

.152 

-.365* 

5. ADBP 





— 

.056 

6. ASBP 






— 


•g < .05 


Table 5 

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Post COo Fear 

QLzl2} 


Variable 

B 

SEB 

P 

Anxiety Sensitiyity Index (ASI) 

.07 

.02 

.36** 

Body Vigilance Scale (BVS) 

-.03 

.02 

-.15 

Heart Beat Accuracy (HBA) 

.00 

.01 

-.06 

Heart Rate Reactiyity (AHR) 

.03 

.02 

.18 

Diastolic Blood Pressure Reactivity (ADBP) 

.02 

.02 

.14 

Systolic Blood Pressure Reactiyity (ASBP) 

.01 

.02 

.06 


Note. R~= .13 for Step 1 fp < .01) 


♦*E<.01 


Main Effects 

The anxiety sensitiyity index (ASI), body vigilance scale (BVS), heartbeat accuracy 
index (HBA), diastolic blood pressure reactivity (ADBP), systolic blood pressure 
reactivity (ASBP), heart rate reactivity (AHR) were entered hierarchically in the order 



22 


indicated in Table 5. The ASI was the only significant predictor, accounting for 13.2% of 
the variance in self-reported fear after the CO 2 challenge. Adding each of the other 
predictors did not significantly increase the variance explained. 


Table 6 


Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Cross Products Predicting Post CO; 


Fear (N = 72 ) 


Variable 

B 

^B 

3 

Step 1 

Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI) 

.079 

.774 

.44** 

Body Vigilance Scale (BVS) 

-.031 

.022 

-.17 ■ 

Heart Beat Accuracy (HBA) 

.000 

.021 

.00 

Heart Rate Reactivity (AHR) 

.033 

.008 

.19 

Diastolic Blood Pressure Reactivity (ADBP) 

.021 

.021 

.13 

Systolic Blood Pressure Reactivity (ASBP) 

.009 

.020 

.06 

Step 2 

ASIxBVS 

.000 

.002 

.02 

Step 3 

ASIxHBA 

.000 

.001 

-.23 

Step 4 

ASIxAHR 

.007 

.003 

.58* 

Step 5 

ASIxADBP 

.005 

.003 

.54* 

Step 6 

ASIxASBP 

.004 

.002 

.42 


Note. = .21 for Step 1; AR^ = .05 for Step 4; AR^ = .05 for Step 5 (gs < .05) 


''‘p< .05. •*£< .01. 


Interaction Effects 


Table 6 represents the second multiple regression analysis examining the effects of 



23 


the cross products of the ASI and the other five independent variables. ASIxAHR cross 
product (Step 4) increased the value to .266, a significant improvement over Step 3, 
indicating anxiety sensitivity’s effect on fear is not the same for different levels of heart 
rate reactivity. A graphical representation of the ASI/AHR interaction is depicted in 
Figure 1. 

Similarly, the ASIxADBP cross product significantly increased the R^ value to .312. 
A graphical representation of the ASIxADBP interaction is depicted in Figure 2. The 
addition of the other cross products did not contribute significantly to the model. The 
overall model explained 34.5% of the variance in post-COi self-reported fear. 

Discussion 

Cognitive notions of panic assert the preeminence of catastrophic thought processes 
in the development and maintenance of panic, and the findings from the present study 
lend support to this notion. When considering anxiety sensitivity, cardiovascular 
reactivity, body vigilance, and interoceptive accuracy, anxiety sensitivity emerged as the 
only significant factor associated with self-reported fear of carbon dioxide-induced 
arousal. These results suggest anxiety sensitivity to be a dispositional factor that 
contributes to fearful responding to symptoms of arousal independent of panic history. 
Additionally, the findings are consistent with the formulation that when one perceives 
arousal as threatening, one is more apt to respond fearfully to arousal-inducing agents and 
activities regardless of one’s prior history of panic attacks. These findings have been 
reported in previous studies investigating the role of anxiety sensitivity in nonclinical 



24 

subjects exposed to biological challenges (Holloway and McNally, 1987; Schmidt and 
Telch, 1994; Telch et al., 1996). 





The remaining results will be discussed in light of Ehlers’s (1993) hypotheses 
regarding the role of heightened somatic perception in panic, namely increased vigilance, 
enhanced interoceptive acuity, and greater physiological reactivity. In the current, 
nonclinical sample, increased vigilance did not emerge as a significant factor associated 
with self-reported fear, neither singularly nor in interaction with anxiety sensitivity. The 
failure of vigilance to be associated with fearful responding to autonomic arousal 
suggests that vigilance, although potentially important in the maintenance of panic, is 
unlikely to be a substantial contributor to the development of panic. 

The results suggest a similar conclusion with regard to interoceptive acuity. 

Although accuracy in detecting somatic changes may foster and sustain panicogenic 
cognitions and behaviors, the current findings do not implicate it as a significant 
contributor to fearful responding in subjects without histories of panic. 

As for Ehlers’s third hypothesis, that individuals who experience panic are more 
physiologically reactive, the current findings are more encouraging. Although none of 
the cardiovascular reactivity measures (i.e., heart rate, diastolic blood pressure, systolic 
blood pressure) singularly contributed to self-reported fear, heart rate reactivity and 
diastolic blood pressure reactivity, when considered with anxiety sensitivity status, 
appears to be a significant factor. As hypothesized, an anxiety sensitivity and diastolic 
blood pressure reactivity interaction emerged as a significant contributor to fearful 
responding to the carbon dioxide challenge, indicating high heart rate and diastolic blood 
pressure reactivity may increase fearful responding in individuals who also have high 
anxiety sensitivity. Because these findings emerged in this nonclinical sample, the results 
suggest these factors to be potential risks for the development of panic. This finding is 



26 


consistent with previous reports from ambulatory monitoring of individuals with panic 
disorder showing increased diastolic blood pressure reactivity during and near panic 
(Shear et al., 1992; White and Baker, 1987; Bystritsky et al., 1995) and with reports of 
elevated diastolic blood pressure in individuals with panic disorder in response to CO 2 
challenge. 

Additionally, a trend towards an anxiety sensitivity and systolic blood pressure 
interaction emerged in the analyses (AR^ = .033, p < .09), indicating the potential role of 
systolic blood pressure reactivity or the potential role of a general measure of reactivity 
(e.g., mean arterial pressure) that should be investigated in future studies with greater 
statistical power. 

Limitations of the Study 

The primary limitations of this study were the small sample size, missing data, and 
the cross-sectional design. The small sample size was a direct result of the methodology 
used for recruitment of subjects. Although random digit dialing techniques theoretically 
render a more representative sample, people today are inundated with tele-marketers and 
phone solicitors, so much so that many households were unwilling to speak to us and 
unwilling to return our phone calls. This method of subject recruitment could have 
resulted in an additional bias in that, intuitively, it seems that more anxious people would 
be less willing to commit to driving across a large metropolitan area to participate in a 
research study. 

To some extent, this bias is supported by the sample’s low scores relative to other 
nonclinical samples on the anxiety sensitivity index (McNally, 1996), and the body 
vigilance scale (Schmidt et al., 1997). Additionally, the sample’s high degree of 



27 


inaccuracy in detecting heart rates, and lower than average baseline blood pressures and 
heart rates (Guyton and Hall, 1996) are indicative of such a selection bias. Subjects’ low 
level of fearful responding to the CO 2 challenge could have also been influenced by the 
selection bias and other methodological limitations. For example, Telch and colleagues 
(1996) found that the emotional responding of nonclinical subjects with high anxiety 
sensitivity is influenced by perceived control (i.e., their ability to regulate their CO 2 
intake). 

In our study, subjects were told they could leave the study at any time, and they 
would be paid for the portion of the protocol they completed. This could have resulted in 
diminished emotional responding in an already nonanxious sample. The low scores on 
the SUDS scale indicating fear of the CO 2 challenge are suggestive of this fact (M = I -2, 
SD = 1.9, possible range, 0-10). The recruitment methodology also resulted in a small 
sample size, which significantly diminished the statistical power of the analyses. 

Missing data points were another source of concern in the current study. Due to the 
small sample size and the wide distribution of missing values, it was not possible to 
discard subjects with missing data. No single subject was missing more than 2 
independent variable measures, but 24 subjects were missing at least one independent 
variable. Substitution with the mean was implemented for these subjects. 

Conclusions 

Although many questions remain regarding the genesis of panic and other anxiety 
disorders, the results of this study, interpreted in light of results from other nonclinical 
examples, have potential theoretical ramifications for current theory. The present 
findings suggest improved accuracy for detecting one’s heart rate and vigilance for 



28 


symptoms of arousal may be common sequelae, but appear to be neither necessary nor 
sufficient links in the anxiety pathology chain. Prospective studies of nonclinical 
subjects will best be able to pursue these findings further. This study additionally 
provides added support to the notion that aberrant, catastrophic cognitions regarding the 
meaning of somatic symptoms may be necessary, and at times sufficient, to induce 
fearful responding to such sensations. As well, an individual’s physiology, specifically 
one’s cardiovascular reactivity, may be an important mediator in the genesis of fear. 



29 


References 

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diaimostic and statistical manual of 
mental disorders (4“* ed.). Washington, DC: Author. 

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of 
mental disorders (S*^** ed.). Washington, DC: Author. 

Antony, M.M., Meadows, E.A., Brown, T.A., and Barlow, D.H., (1994). Cardiac 
awareness before and after cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic disorder. Journal of 
Anxiety Disorders. 8(41. 341-350. 

Asmundson, G.J.G., Norton, G.R., Wilson, K.G., and Sandler, L.S. (1994). 
Subjective symptoms and cardiac reactivity to brief hyperventilation in individuals with 
high anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 32. 237-241. 

Asmundson, G.J.G., Sandler, L.S., Wilson, K.G., and Walker, J.R. (1992). 
Selective attention toward physical threat in patients with panic disorder. Journal of 
Anxiety Disorders. 6, 295-303. 

Barlow, D.H. (1988). Anxiety and its disorders. New York: Guilford. 

Braune, S., Albus, M., Froehler, M., Hoehn, T. et al. (1994). Psychophysiological 
and biochemical changes in patients with panic attacks in a defined situational arousal. 
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 244( 2). 86-92. 

Bums, L.E. and Thorpe, G.L. (1977). The epidemiology of fears and phobias 
(with particular reference to the National Survey of Agoraphobics). International Medical 
Research. 5( suppn. 1-7. 

Bystritsky, A. and Shapiro, D. (1992). Continuous psychological changes and 
subjective reports in panic patients: A preliminary methodological report. Biological 
Psychiatry. 32. 766-777. 



30 

Bystritsky, A., Craske, M., Maidenberg, E., Vapnik, T., and Shapiro, D. (1995). 
Ambulatory monitoring of panic patients during regular activity: A preliminary report. 
Biological Psychiatry. 38, 648-689. 

Clark, D.M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and 
Therapy. 24, 461-470. 

Clark, D.M. and Fairbum, C.G. (1997). Science and practice of cognitive 
behaviour therapy. New York: Oxford. 

Donnell, C.D., and McNally, R.J. (1989). Anxiety sensitivity and history of panic 
as predictors of response to hyperventilation. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 27. 325- 
332. 

Donnell, C.D., and McNally, R.J. (1990). Anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks in 
a nonclinical population. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 28. 83-85. 

Ehlers, A. (1993). Interoception and panic disorder. Advances in Behaviour 
Research and Therapy. 15. 3-21. 

Ehlers, A. (1995). A 1-year prospective study of panic attacks: Clinical course 
and factors associated with maintenance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 104( 0. 164- 
172. 

Ehlers, A., and Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. 
Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 101 (3k 371-382. 

Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Roth, W.T., Taylor,C.B., and Birbaumer, N. (1988). 
Anxiety induced by false heart rate feedback in patients with panic disorder. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy. 26. l-ll. 



31 


Eichler, S. and Katkin, E.S. (1994). The relationship between cardiovascular 
reactivity and heartbeat detection. Psychophysiology. 31. 229-234. 

Eke, M. and McNally, R.J. (1996). Anxiety sensitivity, suffocation fear, and 
breath-holding duration as predictors of response to carbon dioxide challenge. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy. 34 (8), 603-607. 

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J.B.W. (1996). Structured 
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). 
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 

Friedman, B.H., Thayer, J.F., Borkoved, T.D., Tyrrell, R.A., Johnson, B.H., and 
Columbo, R. (1993). Autonomic characteristics of nonclinical panic and blood phobia. 
Biological Psychiatry. 34 (5J. 298-310. 

Friedman, B.H. and Thayer, J.F. (1998). Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: A 
cardiovascular approach. Biological Psychology. 47 (3T 243-263. 

Gorman, J.M., Papp, L.A., Martinez, J., Goetz, R.R., Hollander, E., Liebowitz, 
M.R., and Jordan, F. (1990). High-dose carbon dioxide challenge test in anxiety disorder 
patients. Biological Psychiatry. 28. 743-757. 

Guyton, A.C. and Hall, J.E. (1996). Textbook of medical physiology (9*** ed.V 
W.B. Sanders: Philadelphia. 

Hoehn-Saric, R. and McLeod, D.R. (1988). Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 
li(2), 375-386. 

Hoehn-Saric, R., McLeod, D.R., and Zimmerli, W.D. (1991). 
Psychophysiological response patterns in panic disorder. Acta Psvchiatrica Scandinavica, 
^ 4 - 11 . 



32 


Holloway, W. and McNally, R.J. (1987). Effects of anxiety sensitivity on the 
response to hyperventilation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 96. 330-334. 

Horenstein, M. and Segui, J. (1997). Chronometrics of attentional processes in 
anxiety disorders. Psvchopatholoev. 30( 1). 25-35. 

Katkin, E.S. (1985). Blood, sweat, and tears; Individual differences in autonomic 
self-perception. Psychophysiology. 22 ( 2 ). 125-137. 

Knight, R.G., Waal-Manning, H.J., and Spears, G.F. (1983). Some norms and 
reliability data for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Zung Self-Rating Depression 
scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 245-249. 

Mailer, R.G. and Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety sensitivity in 1984 and panic attacks in 
1987. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 6 f3J. 241-247. 

McLeod, D.R. and Hoehn-Saric, R. (1993). Perception of physiological changes 
in normal and pathological anxeity. In R. Hoehn-Saric and D.R. McLeod (Eds.) Biology 
of Anxiety Disorders (pp. 223-243). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 

McNally, R.J. (1994). Panic disorder: A critical analysis. New York: Guilford. 

McNally, R.J., Amir, N. Louro, C.E., Lukach, B.M., Riemann, B.C., and 
Calamari, J.E. (1994). Cognitive processing of idiographic emotional information in 
panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 32 f IL 119-122. 

McNally, R.J. and Lorenz, M. (1987). Anxiety sensitivity in agoraphobics. 
Journal of Behavior Research and Experimental Psychiatry. 18. 3-11. 

McNally, R.J., Riemann, B.C., and Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing of threat 
cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 28( 5). 407-412. 



33 


Montgomery, W.A., and Jones, G.E. (1984). Laterality, emotionality, and 
heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology. 21. 459-465. 

Norton, G.R., Harrison, B., Hauch, J., and Rhodes, L. (1985). Characteristics of 
people with infrequent panic attacks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 94. 216-221. 

Pauli, P., Marquardt, C., Hartl, L., Nutzinger, D.O., Holzl, R., and Strian, F. 
(1991). Anxiety induced by cardiac perceptions in patients with panic attacks: A field 
study. Behayjour Research and Therapy. 29. 137-145. 

Pennebaker, J.W., Gonder-Frederick, L., Cox, D.J., and Hooyer, C.W. (1985). 

The perception of general ys. specific yisceral actiyity and the regulation of health-related 
behayior. Adyances in Behayioral Medicine. 1. 165-198. 

Peterson, R.A., and Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety Sensitiyity Index manual { 2 '"^ ed.). 
Worthington, OH: International Diagnostic Systems. 

Pilkington, N.W., Antony, M.M. and Swinson, R.P. (1998, Noyember). Vigilance 
for autonomic and non-autonomic bodily sensations across the anxiety disorders and in 
non-clinical yolunteers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the 
Adyancement of Behayior Therapy, Washington, DC. 

Rapee, R.M. and Medoro, L. (1994). Fear of physical sensations and trait anxiety 
as mediators of the response to hyperyentilation in nonclinical subjects. Journal of 
Abnormal Psychology. 103. 693-699. 

Rapee, R.M., Brown, T.A., Antony, M.M, and Barlow, D.H. (1992). Response to 
hyperyentilation and inhalation of 5.5% carbon dioxide-enriched air across the DSM-III- 
R anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 101. 538-552. 





34 


Reiss, S., and McNally, R.J. (1985). The expectancy model of fear. In S. Reiss 
and R.R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 107-121). New 
York: Academic Press. 

Sandler, L.S., Wilson, K.G., Asmundson, G.J., Larsen, D.K., et al. (1992). 
Cardiovascular reactivity in nonclinical subjects with infrequent panic attacks. Journal of 
Anxiety Disorders. 6(11. 27-39. 

Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. 
Psychophysiology, 18(41 . 483-488. 

Schandry, R. (1983). On the relationship between the improvement of cardiac 
perception and the increase of emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 20f 4L 468. 

Schmidt, N.B., and Telch, M.J. (1994). The role of safety information and fear of 
bodily sensations in moderating responses to a hyperventilation challenge. Behavior 
Therapy, 25. 197-208. 

Schmidt, N.B., Lerew, D.R., and Jackson, R.J. (1997). The role of anxiety 
sensitivity in the pathogenesis of panic: Prospective evaluation of spontaneous panic 
attacks during acute stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106( 31. 355-364. 

Schmidt, N.B., Lerew, D.R., and Trakowski, J.H. (1997). Body vigilance in panic 
disorder: Evaluating attention to bodily perturbations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 
Psychology, 65 ( 2 ). 214-220. 

Shands, H.C. and Schor, N. (1982). The modem syndrome of phobo-phobia and 
its management. In R.L. Dupont (Ed.), Phobia: A comprehensive summary of modem 
treatments (pp. 107-116). New York: Brunner/Mazel. 



35 


Shear, M.K., Polan, J.J., Harshfield, G., Pickering, T., Mann, J., France, A. and 
Janies, G. (1992). Ambulatory monitoring of blood pressure and heart rate in panic 
patients. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 6. 213-221. 

Shostak, B.B. and Peterson, R.A. (1990). Effects of anxiety sensitivity on 
emotional response to a stress task. Behavior Research and Therapy. 28. 513-521. 

Stein, J.M., Papp, L.A., Klein, D.F., Cohen, S., Simon, J, Ross, D., Martinez, J. 
and Gorman, J.M. (1992). Exercise tolerance in panic disorder patients. Biological 
Psychiatry. 32. 281-287. 

Steptoe, A. and Noll, A. (1997). The perception of bodily sensations with special 
reference to hypochondriasis. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 35(10). 901-910. 

Sturges, L.V. and Goetsch, V.L. (1996). Psychophysiological reactivity and 
heartbeat awareness in anxiety sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 10(41. 283-294. 

Taylor, C.B., King, R., Ehlers, A., Margraf, J., Clark, D., Hayward, C., Roth, 
W.T. and Agras, S. (1987). Treadmill exercise test and ambulatory measures in panic 
attacks. American Journal of Cardiology. 60. 48J-52J. 

Taylor, S., Koch, W.J., and McNally, R.J. (1992). How does anxiety sensitivity 
vary across the anxiety disorders? Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 6. 249-259. 

Telch, M.J. and Harrington, P.J. (1992, November). The role of anxiety 
sensitivity and expectedness of arousal in mediating affective response to 35% carbon 
dioxide. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior 
Therapy, Boston. 




36 


Telch, M.J., Lucas, J.A., Schmidt, N.B., Hanna, H.H., Jaimez, T.L., and Lucas, 
R.A. (1993). Group cognitive-behavioral treatment of panic disorder. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy. 31. 279-287. 

Telch, M.J., Shermis, T.S., and Lucas, J.A. (1989). Anxiety sensitivity: Unitary 
personality trait or domain-specific appraisals? Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 3(1), 35-32. 

Telch, M.J., Silverman, A., and Schmidt, N.B. (1996). Effects of anxiety 
sensitivity and perceived control on emotional responding to caffeine challenge. Journal 
of Anxiety Disorders. 10(1). 21-35. 

Tyrer, P.J. (1973). Relevance of bodily feelings in emotion. Lancet. 915-916. 

Tyrer, P.J. (1976). The role of bodily feelings in anxiety. London: Oxford 
University Press. 

Tyrer, P.J., Lee, 1., and Alexander, J. (1980). Awareness of cardiac function in 
anxious, phobic, and hypochondrical patients. Psychological Medicine. 10, 171-174. 

Van Zijderveld, G., TenVoorde, B.J., Veltman, D.J., and van Doomen, L.J.P. 
(1997). Cardiovascular, respiratory and panic reactions to epinephrine in panic disorder 
patients. Biological Psychiatry. 41 f2L 249-251. 

Veltman, D.J., van Zijderveld, G., Tilders, F.J.H., and van Dyck, R. (1996). 
Epinephrine and fear of bodily sensations in panic disorder and social phobia. Journal of 
Psvchopharmacologv. 10( 41. 259-265. 

Weissman, M.M. and Merikangas, K.R. (1986). The epidemiology of anxiety and 
panic disorders, an update. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 4 7 (suppl), 11-17. 

White, W.B. and Baker, H.B. (1987). Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring in 
patients with panic disorder. Archives of Internal Medicine. 147. 1973-1975. 



37 


Whitehead, W.E., Drescher, V.M., Heiman, P., and Blackwell, B. (1977). 

Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation. 

2, 371-392. 

Williams, J.B., Spitzer, R.L., and Gibbon, M. (1992). International reliability of a 
diagnostic intake procedure for panic disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 149( 41. 
560-562. 

Yeregani, V.K., Meiri, P.C., Pohl, R., Balon, R., Desai, N. and Golec, S. (1990). 
Heart rate and blood pressure changes during postural change and isometric handgrip 
exercise in patients with panic disorder and normal controls. Acta Psvchiatrica 
Scandinavica. 83. 4-11. 

Zoellner, L.A. and Craske, M.G. (1999). Interoceptive accuracy and panic attacks. 
Behaviour Research and Therapy. 37 (12). 1141-1158.